• October 1, 2022
  • No Comment
  • 87 minutes read

Carcass detection and consumption by facultative scavengers in forest ecosystem highlights the value of their ecosystem services | Scientific Reports – Nature.com

Carcass detection and consumption by facultative scavengers in forest ecosystem highlights the value of their ecosystem services | Scientific Reports – Nature.com

Thank you for visiting nature.com. You are using a browser version with limited support for CSS. To obtain the best experience, we recommend you use a more up to date browser (or turn off compatibility mode in Internet Explorer). In the meantime, to ensure continued support, we are displaying the site without styles and JavaScript.
Advertisement
Scientific Reports volume 12, Article number: 16451 (2022)
Metrics details
Scavenging is a common feeding behavior that provides ecosystem services by removing potentially infectious waste from the landscape. The importance of facultative scavenging is often overlooked, but likely becomes especially important in ecosystems without obligate scavengers. Here, we investigated the ecological function of vertebrate facultative scavengers in removing ungulate carcasses from Japanese forests that lack obligate scavengers. We found that mammals detected carcasses first more often than birds, and that raccoon dogs (Nyctereutes procyonoides) were the most frequent scavenger to first detect carcasses. However, we found no evidence of there being species that signal the location of carrion to other species via social cues. Instead, higher temperatures promoted earlier detection of the carcasses by scavengers, likely related to increased olfactory signals. The carcasses were completely consumed on average in 7.0 days, reasonably similar to other systems regardless of habitat, indicating that facultative scavengers are providing ecosystem services. Larger carcasses tended to take longer to deplete, but carcasses were consumed faster in warmer temperatures. Our results indicate that facultative scavengers were capable of consuming carrion and contributing ecosystem services in a forest ecosystem that lacks obligate scavengers.
Animals who scavenge provide structure and important ecosystem services to ecological communities1,2,3. For example, scavengers provide ecosystem services by removing carcasses that are a source of harmful pathogens from ecosystems1,4,5. These scavenger species range from invertebrates to vertebrates, each of which compete for ephemeral carrion1,3. Scavenger species are often diverse with different traits and can fill many different ecological roles within communities.
Within the vertebrate scavenger community, obligate scavengers (species, like vultures, that are totally dependent on carrion6) are key species in scavenging networks that make critical contributions to its ecological function7. Obligate scavengers are excellent at detecting carrion, as a result, lead to carcasses being consumed faster8,9. Specifically, while obligate scavengers are highly efficient at consuming carrion, they also promote the interspecific interactions with other scavengers by signaling carcass locations while also opening large carcasses and allowing other scavengers access, thereby enhancing the carcass consumption rate6,10,11. But scavenging by facultative scavengers (species that opportunistically feed on carrion such as carnivora and raptors12) is also widespread, even in the absence of vultures (e.g., Refs.13,14). Facultative scavengers also include some key species, with high olfactory acuity and social foragers, playing an important ecological role in the facilitation of carrion consumption in a scavenger community11. Thus, it is important to understand ecosystem function (i.e., carrion consumption) by the facultative scavenger community in a system where obligate scavengers are absent. Nevertheless, there are still limited studies evaluating vertebrate facultative scavenger communities (e.g., Refs.15,16).
Japanese forest ecosystem lacks obligate scavengers (i.e., vultures), and instead the scavenger community is composed of facultative scavengers, dominated by raccoon dogs (Nyctereutes procyonoides) and Asian black bears (Ursus thibetanus) that are the primary scavengers of large ungulate carcass such as sika deer (Cervus nippon; hereafter “deer”)17,18. Therefore, facultative scavengers could be especially important to ecosystem services in this system through removing carrion16,19,20. On the other hand, invertebrates are also active in this system except in winter. Vertebrates and invertebrates are known to compete for carrion especially warm temperatures21,22, and invertebrates may perform functional compensation of vertebrates, as evidenced by the consumption of house mouse Mus musculus (Muridae) in the insular system23. It is unclear whether facultative vertebrate scavengers contribute to the removal of deer carcasses in addition to invertebrates, and or whether there are species in the system (similar to vultures) that signal carcass locations to other scavengers. To understand the ecological role of vertebrate scavenging, it is important to evaluate how facultative scavengers detect and consume animal carcasses and consequently contribute to the removal of these carcasses.
Our objectives are to determine the scavenging patterns in (1) the detection time (the elapsed time between the placement of the carcass and the arrival of the first scavenger) and (2) the depletion time (the elapsed time between carcass placement and its complete consumption) of deer carcasses in a Japanese forest ecosystem, and to evaluate the factors that affect these scavenging patterns. We tested the following hypotheses:
Pattern of carcass detection time. We hypothesized that the carcass detection time would differ between mammals and birds, as well as among vertebrate scavenger species depending on their ecological traits. We predicted that mammalian scavengers would detect carcasses first more frequently than avian scavengers due to the limited aerial visual aspects in closed canopy forests (see details in “Materials and methods” section), and specifically that raccoon dogs and Asian black bears would detect carcasses first more frequently than other scavengers due to their more frequent use of carrion17.
Key species signaling the carcass location to other species. We hypothesized the presence of at least one key species that would signal the presence of carcasses to other scavengers. We predicted that this species detects carcasses first, and that carcasses detected by the key species leads to faster detections for other scavengers.
Factors affecting the carcass detection time for the scavenging community. We hypothesized that carcass detection time would be affected by olfactory and visual cues. We predicted that first detection time for each carcass would be different based on temperature and the presence of understory vegetation (Table 1).
Pattern of the carcass depletion time. We hypothesized that the carcass depletion time would be long due to the absence of obligate scavengers and given closed canopy forests cause delays in detection and consumption by avian scavengers. We predicted that the carcass depletion time would be one of the longest reported in the literature (e.g., > 21 days18).
Factors affecting the carcass depletion time for the scavenging community. We hypothesized that the carcass depletion time would be affected by the size of the carcass and competition with invertebrate scavenging. We predicted that carcass depletion time would be negatively affected by carcass weight and positively affected by temperature (Table 1).
The mean detection time of mammalian and avian scavengers were 4.9 days ± 4.2 SD and 4.7 days ± 3.7 SD respectively. Mammalian scavengers detected most carcasses first (88.6%), and significantly more often than avian scavengers (11.4%, p < 0.001; Table 2). Raccoon dogs most often detected the carcasses first (40.9%), significantly more frequent than other species (p < 0.001; Table 2), partially supporting hypothesis 1a. Masked palm civets (Paguma larvata) and black kites (Milvus migrans) never detected carcasses first, which was significantly less than other species (pmasked palm civet = 0.009, pblack kite = 0.009; Table 2). Raccoon dogs also had the fastest mean detection time (3.3 d ± 3.5 SD, p = 0.004; Table 2). In contrast, masked palm civets (9.8 days ± 4.8 SE, p = 0.004; Table 2) had the slowest mean detection time.
We considered whether raccoon dogs could be a key species for carcass detection, and raccoon dogs detecting carcasses first did not significantly affect the detection times for other scavengers (β = 0.266; p < 0.017), not supporting hypothesis 1b. Meanwhile, the first detection times for carcasses were significantly decreased by temperature (β = − 0.119; p < 0.001) but not influenced by the presence of understory vegetation (β = − 0.204; p = 0.472, Fig. 1), partially supporting hypothesis 1c.
The predicted effects of temperature and understory vegetation on the detection time for ungulate carcasses in a GLM. Points represent measured values, lines represent mean estimates, and shaded regions represent 95% confidence intervals. We calculated the relationships while keeping other independent variables constant (temperature is set to the mean, understory vegetation is set to the reference level).
The mean carcass depletion time was 7.0 days, and ranged from 2.4 to 20.6 days. The survival analysis showed that 97% of carcasses survived to 2.4 days and 96% of carcasses were completely consumed within 16.5 days (Fig. 2). This carcass depletion time ranks 18th of 26 studies reviewed in the literature comparison (Ref.18; Fig. S1). But our study was the second fastest depletion time among seven studies without obligate scavengers, and also 8th fastest depletion time among 15 studies conducted primarily in forests (Fig. S1), failing to support hypothesis 2a.
The probability of deer carcass persistence (n = 44) with corresponding 95% confidence intervals. The cross key shows the censored point in which carcass consumption had not been fully observed (n = 9).
The depletion times were significantly decreased by the temperature (β = − 0.072; p < 0.001, Fig. 3). The carcass weight did not significantly affect the depletion times but had a positive effect (β = 0.005; p = 0.269, Fig. 3), partially supporting hypothesis 2b.
The predicted effects of temperature and carcass weight for carcass depletion time in a GLM. Points represent measured values, lines represent mean estimates, and shaded regions represent 95% confidence intervals. We calculated the relationships while keeping other independent variables constant (temperature and carcass weight are set to the mean).
How quickly a scavenger can detect a carcass is an important aspect of their ability to acquire nutrition from carrion6. In our forest ecosystem most of the carcasses (88.6%) were first detected by facultative mammalian scavengers. This suggests that carcass detection by avian scavengers was likely restricted in the forest ecosystem17, and was apparently slower in the absence of obligate scavengers. This system was also missing large predators that kill adult ungulates, which may reduce the ability of avian scavengers to detect carcasses, as avian scavengers efficiently detect and utilize carcasses by tracking large predators24. However, because most mammals have good olfactory senses, they can detect carcasses themselves when searching for food even in the absence of the species that promote carcass detection.
The carcass detection ability of each species is not necessarily proportional to their scavenging frequency and instead it may depend on specific traits, dependence on nutrition from carrion, and their population density. Raccoon dogs, the most frequent scavenger, were the first scavenger at nearly half of the carcasses, while Asian black bears were the second most frequent scavenger but did not detect carcasses significantly earlier than other species. Masked palm civets and black kites, which are the least frequent scavenger17, also never detected carcasses first. Although raccoon dogs first detected carcasses significantly more often than other species, their detection did not lead to faster detections of other scavengers, suggesting they do not play a role in promoting carcass acquisition for the scavenger guild. This lack of species that signal the location of carrion to other species may be one of the key aspects missing in the community due to the lack of obligate scavengers. As for raccoon dogs, the population density in this study area appears higher than other species, and this may have led to their earlier carcass detections (Table S1).
We found that temperature noticeably affected the carcass detection time by vertebrate facultative scavengers. The activities of invertebrate scavengers and decomposers (fungi and microorganisms) accelerate as the temperature rises leading to more rapid carcass decomposition1,25. The resulting odor associated with carcass decomposition spreads, and scavengers are often able detect the carcasses more easily8,26,27. Our results support the results of previous studies that have shown the importance of olfactory cues and temperature to affect scavengers’ ability to find the mammalian carcass8,26,27. In contrast, the absence of understory vegetation had no effect on the detection time by facultative scavengers. This implies that visual cues are not as important for carcass detection in this system, which is contrary to the results of previous studies28,29, although the importance of visual cues often decreases in more closed habitats30,31. This might be because facultative mammalian scavengers put less emphasis on visual cues to detect carcasses than avian scavengers, and the richness of avian scavengers was inherently low17. Furthermore, the understory vegetation might also provide shelter to reduce some risks (e.g., competition with larger species, including humans).
Despite the lack of obligate scavengers in the system, we found that carcasses were completely consumed on average in about a week. This rank is not among the slowest compared to other studies of scavenging on ungulates where scavenging communities contained obligate scavengers. This remained even if limited to studies primarily conducted in forest ecosystems from the result that 8th depletion time among 15 studies. Together, these results suggest that our system made up entirely of facultative scavengers can still provide ecosystem services by removing carrion from the ecosystem. In our study area, this may be dependent on the most frequent scavengers (raccoon dogs and Asian black bears17), which were also the primary consumers of carrion. Another noteworthy point was that it was the second fastest carcass depletion time among study systems without obligate scavengers, and in many systems without obligate scavengers it takes > 1 week for ungulates to be consumed (Fig. S1). The depletion time in our system provides important data showing it had comparatively faster depletion time despite the community being composed entirely of facultative scavengers.
The carcass depletion time was strongly influenced by the temperature. Considering that carcass detection by vertebrates also appears dependent on temperature and odor, there is a tradeoff in the competitive interactions between invertebrate and vertebrate scavengers for finding and exploiting a carcass21,22,32. Specifically, during warmer temperatures, a facultative scavenger can detect the carcass earlier but they must compete more with invertebrates. In contrast, during colder temperatures facultative scavengers compete much less with invertebrates and can scavenge for longer durations, but they cannot detect the carcass as easily or quickly. While we did not directly measure the scavenging by invertebrates in the system, they undoubtedly play a role in carrion consumption and removal of waste from the system. Our result also showed that carcass depletion time tended to increase with carcass weight, but this was not a significant effect. This suggests that competition between vertebrate and invertebrates is a more important factor in carcass depletion. Future studies are necessary to quantify scavenging by all classes of organisms to fully understand the mechanisms of scavenger contributions to ecosystem stability.
We found the facultative scavenger community, composed primarily of omnivorous mammals in our Asian temperate forest ecosystem, was capable of consuming carrion and providing ecosystem services in the system. Our results point to the importance of carrion for nutrition in mammalian carnivores, not just obligate scavengers, and highlight that facultative scavenging is widespread and fills important ecological roles (e.g., by providing ecosystem services; Ref.1,4,5) while also forming weak food web links (e.g., Ref.3,16). While we assumed the absence of obligate scavengers and in closed canopy forests could slow the detection of carcasses due to the social cues vultures provide to the community, we found that facultative scavengers nevertheless consumed carrion relatively quickly (Fig. S1). We also found that detection and consumption by facultative mammalian scavengers are sensitive to environmental factors (e.g., temperature). To understand the stability and maintenance of ecosystems, future research will need to evaluate scavenging interactions within the various biological kingdoms (i.e., vertebrates, invertebrates and microbes) and the abiotic and biotic factors that affect these interactions.
Our research was conducted at Nikko National Park in central Japan (See Ref.17; approximately 1150 km2; 36° 36′ N–37° 05′ N, 139° 19′ E–139° 51′ E). The mean annual temperature was 7.7 °C (− 12.9 to 27.7 °C) and the mean annual rainfall was 2131 mm. The forest types included deciduous broadleaved forests (comprised mainly of Quercus serrata, Q. crispula Blume, and Cerasus jamasakura), conifer plantation forests (comprised mainly Cryptomeria japonica, Chamaecyparis obtuse, and Larix kaempferi), and also patchy mixed forests. The forest floor consisted primarily of bamboo grasses in each forest type, but some places had no understory vegetation due to the foraging pressure of overpopulated deer.
There is no large obligate mammalian predator in the study area, so the carrion sources of large mammals available to vertebrate scavengers are mostly natural-caused deaths (e.g., disease, starvation, and neonatal predation) or human-caused deaths (e.g., culling) rather than predation. The main mammalian scavengers at ungulate carcasses in the system are Asian black bear, wild boar (Sus scrofa), red fox (Vulpes vulpes), raccoon dog, masked palm civet, and Japanese marten (Martes melampus). The main avian scavengers are the jungle crow (Corvus macrorhynchos), black kite, and mountain hawk-eagle (Nisaetus nipalensis)17.
We obtained 44 fresh deer carcasses (nsummer = 19, nautumn = 25), that had not previously been scavenged, from culled nuisance animals or animals killed through vehicle collisions from June to November in 2016 and 2017. We used the culling method that most minimizes pain and distress to the animal, in accordance with the “Welfare and Management of Animals Act” (Ministry of the Environment) and “Specified Wildlife Conservation and Management Plan” (Tochigi Prefecture 2018). We did not receive any ethical approval from the animal ethics committee of Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology as handling of dead wild animal carcasses is not covered by the committee. We performed all handlings of the carcasses according to the guidelines of the American Society of Mammalogists33 and the guidelines for animal research set forth by the Mammal Society of Japan34.
After weighing each deer carcass (and reporting in 10-kg increments), we placed the deer carcasses at randomly selected locations where the canopy was closed and monitored with camera traps (Ltl Acorn 6210, USA) that were programmed to record 30-s videos at each trigger with a 30-s refractory period until > 80% of the carcass including bones and skins had been consumed by a scavenger (see details Ref.17). Each carcass was placed at a distance of > 1 km from other carcasses when we obtained the deer carcasses during the same period. In addition, the time between placing subsequent deer carcasses within a radius of 500 m was > 1 month. We secured the deer carcasses to the nearest tree using wire rope to prevent them from being removed from the view of the camera by scavengers.
We also recorded temperature (°C) and understory vegetation on the day we placed each deer carcass. We corrected the temperature at each carcass site based on the difference in altitude from the weather observatory closest to the site by subtracting or adding 0.55 °C for each 100 m increase or decrease in altitude, respectively, according to the recession ratio of temperature35. We classified the understory vegetation as either 1 (where the cover ratio of plants with a height just over each carcass within a 3-m radius of the carcass > 50%) or 0 (the cover ratio < 50%).
We identified each vertebrate scavenger species and classified vertebrate scavengers as either mammalian or avian classes. We used program R 3.2.436 and considered p-values of < 0.05 to be statistically significant for all of our statistical analyses.
To understand the pattern of carcass detection (Hypothesis 1a), we calculated the mean carcass detection time for each scavenger species. We used Wilcoxon rank sum test37 to test differences in the detection times between each scavenger species with all other scavengers. To identify whether there are species that detect deer carcasses earlier than other species, we first calculated the number of carcasses where each scavenger was the first to detect a carcass. We then used Fisher’s exact tests38 to examine differences in the proportion of first detected carcasses between the classes of mammalian and avian scavengers, and also among scavenger pairs (where we compared each scavenger species against all others).
To determine whether any key species detecting carcasses first lead to faster carcass detections of other scavengers (Hypothesis 1b), we first designated the species with the highest frequency of first detections (raccoon dogs) as the key species. We then developed a generalized linear model (GLM), using the carcass detection times for all scavengers excluding raccoon dogs and the other species with relatively few detections (masked palm civets of eight detections, mountain hawk-eagles of five detections, and black kites of four detections) as the dependent variables. The GLM was best fit to a Gamma distribution with a log link. We used the presence or absence (0 or 1) of raccoon dogs detectiong carcasses first as a independent variable in the model.
To understand the factors that determine carcass detection time for the scavenging community (Hypothesis 1c), we developed a GLM. We used the first detection times for each deer carcass as the dependent variables which were best fit to a Gamma distribution with a log link, and used temperature (°C) and the presence of understory vegetation (0 or 1) as the independent variables (Table 1).
To understand the pattern of carcass depletion time (Hypothesis 2a), we calculated the carcass depletion time for each carcass. We considered the complete carcass consumption when only bones and skin remained or when carcasses eith edible portions were removed from the camera by scavengers18. We used Kaplan–Meier survival analyses using the package “survival”39 and estimated the probability of deer carcass persistence across time. We censored the trials in which we did not monitor the carcass until it was completely consumed (n = 9) due to camera malfunctions or displacement by Asian black bears. We also compared our mean depletion time to the mean depletion times reported by Ref.18 for studies using ungulates that weighed < 100 kg. We classified studies conducted in forest and non-forest areas using Google Maps (Google LLC, USA) and reported latitude and longitude or the study site in the original paper.
To evaluate the factors that affect the carcass depletion time (Hypothesis 2b), we developed a GLM. We used the carcass depletion time as the dependent variable, which was best fit to a Gamma distribution with a log link. We used the independent variables temperature (°C) and carcass weight (kg), in the model.
The data for this manuscript will be submitted to the Illinois Data Bank (https://databank.illinois.edu/).
DeVault, T. L., Rhodes, O. E. & Shivik, J. A. Scavenging by vertebrates: Behavioral, ecological, and evolutionary perspectives on an important energy transfer pathway in terrestrial ecosystems. Oikos 102, 225–234 (2003).
Google Scholar 
Selva, N., Jedrzejewska, B., Jedrzejewski, W. & Wajrak, A. Scavenging on European bison carcasses in Bialowieza Primeval Forest (eastern Poland). Ecoscience 10, 303–311 (2003).
Google Scholar 
Wilson, E. E. & Wolkovich, E. M. Scavenging: How carnivores and carrion structure communities. Trends Ecol. Evol. 26, 129–135 (2011).
PubMed  Google Scholar 
Inger, R., Cox, D. T. C., Per, E., Norton, B. A. & Gaston, K. J. Ecological role of vertebrate scavengers in urban ecosystems in the UK. Ecol. Evol. 6, 7015–7023 (2016).
PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Moleón, M. et al. Humans and scavengers: The evolution of interactions and ecosystem services. Bioscience 64, 394–403 (2014).
Google Scholar 
Moleón, M., Sánchez-Zapata, J. A., Selva, N., Donázar, J. A. & Owen-Smith, N. Inter-specific interactions linking predation and scavenging in terrestrial vertebrate assemblages. Biol. Rev. 89, 1042–1054 (2014).
PubMed  Google Scholar 
Mateo-Tomás, P., Olea, P. P., Moleón, M., Selva, N. & Sánchez-Zapata, J. A. Both rare and common species support ecosystem services in scavenger communities. Glob. Ecol. Biogeogr. 26, 1459–1470 (2017).
Google Scholar 
Houston, D. C. Scavenging efficiency of turkey vultures in tropical forest. Condor 88, 318–323 (1986).
Google Scholar 
Morales-Reyes, Z. et al. Scavenging efficiency and red fox abundance in Mediterranean mountains with and without vultures. Acta Oecol. 79, 81–88 (2017).
ADS  Google Scholar 
Kane, A. & Kendall, C. J. Understanding how mammalian scavengers use information from avian scavengers: Cue from above. J. Anim. Ecol. 86, 837–846 (2017).
PubMed  Google Scholar 
Sebastián-González, E. et al. Functional traits driving species role in the structure of terrestrial vertebrate scavenger networks. Ecology. https://doi.org/10.1002/ecy.3519 (2021).
PubMed  Google Scholar 
Beasley, J. C., Olson, Z. H. & DeVault, T. L. Ecological role of vertebrate scavengers. In Carrion Ecology, Evolution and Their Applications (eds Benbow, M. E. et al.) 107–127 (CRC Press, 2015).
Google Scholar 
Bassi, E., Battocchio, D., Marcon, A., Stahlberg, S. & Apollonio, M. Scavenging on ungulate carcasses in a mountain forest area in Northern Italy. Mamm. Study 43, 1–11 (2018).
Google Scholar 
Enari, H. & Enari, H. S. Not avian but mammalian scavengers efficiently consume carcasses under heavy snowfall conditions: A case from northern Japan. Mamm. Biol. 101, 419–428 (2021).
Google Scholar 
Peers, M. J. L. et al. Prey availability and ambient temperature influence carrion persistence in the boreal forest. J. Anim. Ecol. 89, 2156–2167 (2020).
PubMed  Google Scholar 
Selva, N. & Fortuna, M. A. The nested structure of a scavenger community. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 274, 1101–1108 (2007).
Google Scholar 
Inagaki, A. et al. Vertebrate scavenger guild composition and utilization of carrion in an East Asian temperate forest. Ecol. Evol. 10, 1223–1232 (2020).
PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 
Sebastián-González, E. et al. Network structure of vertebrate scavenger assemblages at the global scale: Drivers and ecosystem functioning implications. Ecography (Cop.) 43, 1143–1155 (2020).
Google Scholar 
Cortés-Avizanda, A., Selva, N., Carrete, M. & Donázar, J. A. Effects of carrion resources on herbivore spatial distribution are mediated by facultative scavengers. Basic Appl. Ecol. 10, 265–272 (2009).
Google Scholar 
Sebastián-González, E. et al. Nested species-rich networks of scavenging vertebrates support high levels of interspecific competition. Ecology 97, 95–105 (2016).
PubMed  Google Scholar 
Beasley, J. C., Olson, Z. H. & Devault, T. L. Carrion cycling in food webs: Comparisons among terrestrial and marine ecosystems. Oikos 121, 1021–1026 (2012).
Google Scholar 
Ray, R. R., Seibold, H. & Heurich, M. Invertebrates outcompete vertebrate facultative scavengers in simulated lynx kills in the Bavarian Forest National Park, Germany. Anim. Biodivers. Conserv. 37, 77–88 (2014).
Google Scholar 
Sugiura, S. & Hayashi, M. Functional compensation by insular scavengers: The relative contributions of vertebrates and invertebrates vary among islands. Ecography (Cop.) 41, 1173–1183 (2018).
Google Scholar 
Wilmers, C. C., Stahler, D. R., Crabtree, R. L., Smith, D. W. & Getz, W. M. Resource dispersion and consumer dominance: Scavenging at wolf- and hunter-killed carcasses in Greater Yellowstone, USA. Ecol. Lett. 6, 996–1003 (2003).
Google Scholar 
Putman, A. R. J. Patterns of carbon dioxide evolution from decaying carrion: Decomposition of small mammal carrion in temperate systems, Part 1. Oikos 31, 47–57 (1978).
CAS  Google Scholar 
DeVault, T. L. & Rhodes, O. E. Identification of vertebrate scavengers of small mammal carcasses in a forested landscape. Acta Theriol. (Warsz.) 47, 185–192 (2002).
Google Scholar 
Selva, N., Jȩdrzejewska, B., Jȩdrzejewski, W. & Wajrak, A. Factors affecting carcass use by a guild of scavengers in European temperate woodland. Can. J. Zool. 83, 1590–1601 (2005).
Google Scholar 
Ogada, D. L., Torchin, M. E., Kinnaird, M. F. & Ezenwa, V. O. Effects of vulture declines on facultative scavengers and potential implications for mammalian disease transmission. Conserv. Biol. 26, 453–460 (2012).
CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 
Turner, K. L., Abernethy, E. F., Conner, L. M., Rhodes, O. E. & Beasley, J. C. Abiotic and biotic factors modulate carrion fate and vertebrate scavenging communities. Ecology 98, 2413–2424 (2017).
PubMed  Google Scholar 
Arrondo, E. et al. Rewilding traditional grazing areas affects scavenger assemblages and carcass consumption patterns. Basic Appl. Ecol. 41, 56–66 (2019).
Google Scholar 
Moleón, M. et al. Carrion availability in space and time. In Carrion Ecology and Management (eds Pedro, P. O. et al.) 23–44 (Springer, 2019).
Google Scholar 
Pereira, L. M., Owen-Smith, N. & Moleón, M. Facultative predation and scavenging by mammalian carnivores: Seasonal, regional and intra-guild comparisons. Mamm. Rev. 44, 44–55 (2014).
Google Scholar 
Animal Care and Use Committee. Guidelines for the capture, handling, and care of mammals as approved by the American Society of Mammalogists. J. Mamm. 79, 1416–1431 (1998).
Google Scholar 
Committee of Reviewing Taxon Names and Specimen Collections. Guidelines for the Procedure of Obtaining Mammal Specimens as Approved by the Mammal Society of Japan (Revised in 2009) (Mammal Society of Japan, 2009).
Google Scholar 
Yoshino, M. Microclimate: New Edition (Chijin Shokan, 1986).
Google Scholar 
R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. https://www.r-project.org/ (2019).
Sokal, R. R. & Rohlf, F. J. Biometry 4th edn. (WH Freeman and Company, 2012).
MATH  Google Scholar 
Fisher, R. A. Statistical Methods for Research Workers (Oliver and Boyd, 1934).
MATH  Google Scholar 
Therneau, T. A Package for Survival Analysis in S. Version 2.38 (2015).
Pardo-Barquín, E., Mateo-Tomás, P. & Olea, P. P. Habitat characteristics from local to landscape scales combine to shape vertebrate scavenging communities. Basic Appl. Ecol. 34, 126–139 (2019).
Google Scholar 
Moleón, M., Sánchez-Zapata, J. A., Sebastián-González, E. & Owen-Smith, N. Carcass size shapes the structure and functioning of an African scavenging assemblage. Oikos 124, 1391–1403 (2015).
Google Scholar 
DeVault, T. L., Brisbin, I. L. & Rhodes, O. E. Factors influencing the acquisition of rodent carrion by vertebrate scavengers and decomposers. Can. J. Zool. 82, 502–509 (2004).
Google Scholar 
Download references
The authors are grateful to Y. Sugimoto and S. Akutsu for his cooperation with field work. They also thank the members of the Asian Black Bear Research Group and the hunting association in Fujiwara division, Tochigi Prefecture for supporting our study. This work was supported partly by a JSPS KAKENHI Grant (No. 16H04932, No. 16H02555, No. 17H05971, No. 21K19858, No. 21J20185), Institute of Global Innovation Research in TUAT, and grants-in-aid of The Inui Memorial Trust for Research on Animal Science.
United Graduate School of Agricultural Science, Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology, 3-5-8 Saiwai-Cho, Fuchu, Tokyo, 183-8509, Japan
Akino Inagaki & Kahoko Tochigi
Illinois Natural History Survey, University of Illinois, Champaign, USA
Maximilian L. Allen
Nature Preservation Division, Tochigi Prefecture, Utsunomiya, Tochigi, Japan
Tetsuya Maruyama
Department of Forest Science, Tokyo University of Agriculture, Setagaya, Tokyo, Japan
Koji Yamazaki
National Agriculture and Food Research Organization, Tsukuba, Ibaraki, Japan
Tomoko Naganuma
Institute of Global Innovation Research, Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology, Fuchu, Japan
Shinsuke Koike
Institute of Agriculture, Tokyo University of Agriculture and Technology, Fuchu, Japan
Shinsuke Koike
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
You can also search for this author in PubMed Google Scholar
A.I. and S.K. designed and coordinated the study. A.I., T.N. and T.M. performed the field work. A.I., K.Y. and K.T. analyzed the data. A.I., M.A. and S.K. wrote the manuscript.
Correspondence to Akino Inagaki.
The authors declare no competing interests.
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article’s Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article’s Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/.
Reprints and Permissions
Inagaki, A., Allen, M.L., Maruyama, T. et al. Carcass detection and consumption by facultative scavengers in forest ecosystem highlights the value of their ecosystem services. Sci Rep 12, 16451 (2022). https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20465-4
Download citation
Received: 17 November 2021
Accepted: 13 September 2022
Published: 30 September 2022
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-022-20465-4
Anyone you share the following link with will be able to read this content:
Sorry, a shareable link is not currently available for this article.

Provided by the Springer Nature SharedIt content-sharing initiative
By submitting a comment you agree to abide by our Terms and Community Guidelines. If you find something abusive or that does not comply with our terms or guidelines please flag it as inappropriate.
Advertisement
Scientific Reports (Sci Rep) ISSN 2045-2322 (online)
© 2022 Springer Nature Limited
Sign up for the Nature Briefing newsletter — what matters in science, free to your inbox daily.

source

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *