- January 23, 2023
- No Comment
- 5 minutes read
Dogs can do more than just tricks; they can even be asked to ponder … – University at Buffalo
News and views for the UB community
Allison Scagel, author of new UB research suggesting dogs can access memories of their actions, with one of the study’s participants, her own dog, Todd. Photo: Meredith Forrest Kulwicki
Published July 18, 2022
Teaching a dog to sit or roll over? That’s easy. But what about that cute head tilt that you’ve never seen before, which happened while your phone was out of reach? Now you want a picture. But how do you get a dog to repeat an action it hasn’t been trained to perform?
For dogs taught to “think back” on cue, you just need to ask. Dogs are capable of learning the instruction “do that again,” and can flexibly access memories of their own recent actions — cognitive abilities they were not known to possess, according to the results of a recent UB study.
“We found that dogs could be trained to repeat specific actions on cue, and then take what they’d learned and apply it to actions they had never been asked to repeat,” says Allison Scagel, the study’s corresponding author, who was a UB graduate student in the Department of Psychology at the time of the research. “Our findings showed that they were able to apply the concept of repetition to new situations. More generally, we found evidence that dogs are capable of forming abstract concepts.”
Historically, there has been a notion that conscious awareness of past personal experiences is the exclusive domain of humans, but recent research isn’t supporting that conclusion, according to Scagel.
“Our study shows that dogs are capable of conceptualization, placing them in an expanding category of other animals that includes bottlenose dolphins and chimpanzees.”
Scagel says the findings, published in the Journal of Comparative Psychology, present new flexible training possibilities for dogs.
“Dogs can do more than learn the relationship between a person’s cue and which specific trick they should perform,” she says. “They can understand the concept of repetition: Whatever you just did, do that again.
“It can apply to anything they do.”
Animals are often tested on their ability to recall things in the external environment they have recently observed, such as objects, sounds or scents. Memories of actions are different because they’re not outwardly perceivable. Memories are entirely internal; they are purely mental representations of previous personal experiences that can be recalled in ways that might influence what an animal chooses to do in the future.
For this study, the researchers looked at dogs’ memories of their own recently performed actions to determine if they could voluntarily think back to what they had just been doing and reproduce those actions.
Three dogs participated: Todd, a male long-haired chihuahua belonging to Scagel; and two female golden retrievers — Aspen, belonging to an acquaintance of Scagel’s, and Layla, belonging to Scagel’s co-author, Eduardo Mercado III, UB professor of psychology.
Traditional dog training is cue and response. When dogs hear or see a trained cue, they respond with a behavior associated with that cue. For a baseline, the researchers started training the dogs in that fashion, with simple cues like spin in a circle, lie down or walk around an object.
The dogs then learned a separate repeat cue — the word “again” accompanied by a hand gesture — that instructed them to reproduce the action they had just completed. To assess whether the dogs had actually learned a general concept of repeating recent actions, they were asked to repeat novel actions that they had never been asked to repeat before. Despite never being trained to repeat these actions, the dogs passed this test.
“This is an important step toward a greater understanding of how other species form abstract concepts,” says Scagel. “And we’re learning that humans aren’t that cognitively unique after all.”
UBNow encourages discussion and welcomes comments from UB faculty, staff and students using a @buffalo.edu email address.
Comments are limited to 125 words and must follow the university’s Comment Guidelines.
The UBNow editor moderates comments and reserves the right not to publish those that do not add anything new to the discussion or fail to adhere to the Comment Guidelines.
Please submit your comments in the box below.
Time quoted LaGarrett King in an article on how some teachers have taken a more nuanced approach to teaching about Martin Luther King Jr.
The Associated Press quoted Amanda Nickerson in a report on the fallout of a shooting at Richneck Elementary School in which a first-grade teacher was shot by a 6-year-old student.